Linguistics: Difference between revisions

From Citizendium
Jump to navigation Jump to search
imported>Howard Arvi Hughes
m (Category:Linguistics Workgroup (Top))
 
imported>John Stephenson
(refresh as of 16th December 2006)
Line 1: Line 1:
{{linguistics}}
{{linguistics}}
'''Linguistics''' is the [[scientific]] study of human [[language]]. Someone who engages in this study is called a '''[[linguist]]'''. Linguistics is conducted along two axes:
'''Linguistics''' is the [[scientific]] study of [[language]]. Someone who engages in this study is called a '''[[linguist]]'''. Linguistics can be theoretical or applied.
* '''Theoretical''' vs. '''Applied''': [[Theoretical linguistics|Theoretical (or general) linguistics]] is concerned with frameworks for describing individual languages and theories about universal aspects of language; [[applied linguistics]] applies these theories to practical problems such as [[language education|language teaching]], [[speech synthesis]], or [[speech therapy]].
* '''Autonomous'''  vs. '''Contextual''' (note: these terms are not well-established): Autonomous linguistics studies what [[Ferdinand de Saussure|Saussure]] called ''langue'' or what [[Noam Chomsky|Chomsky]] calls ''I[nternal]-language'': the nature of language abstracting away from many aspects of its day-to-day usage. Contextual linguistics is concerned with what [[Ferdinand de Saussure|Saussure]] called ''parole'' or what [[Noam Chomsky|Chomsky]] calls ''E[xternal]-language'', namely how language fits into the world: its social function, or its use in the broader context of human behavior.  


Given these distinctions, scholars who call themselves simply ''linguists'' or ''theoretical linguists'', with no further qualification, tend to be concerned with autonomous, theoretical synchronic linguistics, which is acknowledged as the core of the discipline.  
[[Theoretical linguistics|'''Theoretical''' (or '''general''') '''linguistics''']] studies language structure ([[grammar]]), and meaning ([[semantics]]).  The study of grammar encompasses '''[[morphology (linguistics)|morphology]]''' (formation and alteration of [[word]]s) and '''[[syntax]]''' (the rules that determine the way words combine into [[phrase]]s and [[sentence]]s). '''[[Phonology]]''', which is the system used to represent language through abstract '[[phoneme|sounds]]', also forms part of this field.
 
Linguistics compares languages ([[comparative linguistics]]) and explores their histories, in order to find [[language universals|universal properties of language]] and to account for its development and origins ([[historical linguistics]]). Slightly separate from general linguistics is the sub-field of '''[[phonetics]]''', the study of how sounds are produced and perceived.
 
'''[[Applied linguistics]]''' puts linguistic theories into practice in areas such as foreign language teaching, [[speech therapy]], [[translation]] and [[speech pathology]].
Linguistic [[research|inquiry]] is pursued by a wide variety of specialists, who may not all be in harmonious agreement; as [[Russ Rymer]] flamboyantly puts it:
Linguistic [[research|inquiry]] is pursued by a wide variety of specialists, who may not all be in harmonious agreement; as [[Russ Rymer]] flamboyantly puts it:


Line 10: Line 13:


==Divisions, specialties, and subfields==
==Divisions, specialties, and subfields==
The central concern of autonomous theoretical linguistics is to characterize the nature of human linguistic ability, or competence: to explain what it is that an individual knows when an individual knows a language, and to explain how it is that individuals come to know languages. All humans (setting aside extremely pathological cases) achieve competence in whatever language is spoken around them when they are growing up (or signed, in the case of sign language), without formal instruction being necessary. Non-humans do not. Therefore, there is some basic innate property of humans that causes them to learn language. Since children learn whatever language is spoken around them, there is no genetic basis for the differences between one language and another.  
The central concern of theoretical linguistics is to characterize the nature of human language ability, or ''competence'': to explain what it is that an individual ''knows'' when said to know a language; and to explain how it is that individuals ''come to know'' languages.
 
All humans (setting aside extremely pathological cases) achieve competence in whatever language is spoken (or signed, in the case of [[sign language]]) around them when they are [[maturity|growing up]], with apparently little need for conscious instruction. Non-humans do not. Therefore, there is some basic innate property of humans that causes them to be able to use language. There is no discernable ''genetic'' process responsible for differences between languages: an individual will acquire whatever language(s) they are exposed to as a child, regardless of their parentage or ethnic origin.  


Linguistic structures are pairings of meaning and sound (or other externalization). Linguists may specialize in some subpart of the linguistic structure, which can be arranged in the following terms, from sound to meaning:
Linguistic structures are pairings of meaning and sound (or other externalization). Linguists may specialize in some subpart of the linguistic structure, which can be arranged in the following terms, from sound to meaning:
Line 30: Line 35:
* [[Psycholinguistics]], the study of the cognitive processes and representations underlying language use
* [[Psycholinguistics]], the study of the cognitive processes and representations underlying language use
* [[Sociolinguistics]], the study of social patterns of linguistic variability
* [[Sociolinguistics]], the study of social patterns of linguistic variability
* [[Clinical linguistics]], the application of linguistic theory to the area of [[Speech-Language Pathology]]


== Variation ==
== Variation ==
Line 84: Line 90:


Many areas of applied linguistics today involve the explicit use of computers. [[Speech synthesis]] and [[speech recognition]] use phonetic and phonemic knowledge to provide voice interfaces to computers. Applications of [[computational linguistics]] in [[machine translation]], [[computer-assisted translation]], and [[natural language processing]] are extremely fruitful areas of applied linguistics which have come to the forefront in recent years with increasing computing power. Their influence has had a great effect on theories of syntax and semantics, as modelling syntactic and semantic theories on computers constrains the theories to [[Computability theory (computation)|computable]] operations and provides a more rigorous mathematical basis.
Many areas of applied linguistics today involve the explicit use of computers. [[Speech synthesis]] and [[speech recognition]] use phonetic and phonemic knowledge to provide voice interfaces to computers. Applications of [[computational linguistics]] in [[machine translation]], [[computer-assisted translation]], and [[natural language processing]] are extremely fruitful areas of applied linguistics which have come to the forefront in recent years with increasing computing power. Their influence has had a great effect on theories of syntax and semantics, as modelling syntactic and semantic theories on computers constrains the theories to [[Computability theory (computation)|computable]] operations and provides a more rigorous mathematical basis.
Today, the term 'applied linguistics' is used mostly to refer to "second language acquisition."                  [http://www.appliedlinguistics.org Top applied linguistics programs] are usually the ones that have good emphasis on second language acquisition either from linguistic or cognitive point of view.


===Diachronic linguistics===
===Diachronic linguistics===
Line 108: Line 116:
* A number of [[cognitive science|cognitive scientists]] argue that the [[brain]] has an innate "[[language module]]", [[knowledge]] of which is thought to come more from studying speech than writing, particularly since language as speech is held to be an evolutionary adaptation, whereas writing is a comparatively recent invention.
* A number of [[cognitive science|cognitive scientists]] argue that the [[brain]] has an innate "[[language module]]", [[knowledge]] of which is thought to come more from studying speech than writing, particularly since language as speech is held to be an evolutionary adaptation, whereas writing is a comparatively recent invention.


Of course, linguists agree that the study of written language can be worthwhile and valuable. For linguistic research that uses the methods of [[corpus linguistics]] and [[computational linguistics]], written language is often much more convenient for processing large amounts of linguistic data. Large corpora of spoken language are difficult to create and hard to find, and are typically [[transcription (linguistics)|transcribed]] and written.
Of course, linguists agree that the study of written language can be worthwhile and valuable. For linguistic research that uses the methods of [[corpus linguistics]] and [[computational linguistics]], written language is often much more convenient for processing large amounts of linguistic data. Large corpora of spoken language are difficult to create and hard to find, and are typically [[transcription (linguistics)|transcribed]] and written. Additionally, linguists have turned to text-based discourse occurring in various formats of [[computer-mediated communication]] as a viable site for linguistic inquiry.


The study of [[writing systems]] themselves is in any case considered a branch of linguistics.
The study of [[writing systems]] themselves is in any case considered a branch of linguistics.
Line 114: Line 122:
==History of linguistics==
==History of linguistics==
{{Main|History of linguistics}}
{{Main|History of linguistics}}
Early Indian Vedic texts (Rig Veda 1:164:45; 4:58:3; 10:125) suggest a structure for languages: Language is composed of sentences with four stages of evolution that are expressed in three tenses (past, present and future). The sentences are composed of words that have two distinct forms of existence (vocal form, the word, and perceptional form, the meaning). These words are recognized mainly as verbs that represent real world acts and nouns that take on seven* cases (depending on their mode of participation in real world acts). (* The number, seven, here is not very critical; the message is that the nouns are inflected into appropriate cases to indicate their mode of participation in concerned acts).
Early [[History of India|Indian]] [[Vedas|Vedic]] texts (''[[Rigveda|Rig Veda]]'' 1:164:45; 4:58:3; 10:125) suggest a structure for languages: Language is composed of sentences with four stages of evolution that are expressed in three tenses (past, present and future). The sentences are composed of words that have two distinct forms of existence (vocal form, the word, and perceptional form, the meaning). These words are recognized mainly as verbs that represent real world acts and nouns that take on seven* cases (depending on their mode of participation in real world acts). (* The number, seven, here is not very critical; the message is that the nouns are inflected into appropriate cases to indicate their mode of participation in concerned acts).
In [[History of India|ancient India]], the [[Sanskrit]] [[grammarian]] {{Unicode|[[Pāṇini]]}} (c. [[520 BC|520]]–[[460 BC]]) is the earliest known linguist and is often acknowledged as the founder of linguistics. He is most famous for formulating the 3,959 rules of Sanskrit [[Morphology (linguistics)|morphology]] in the text ''{{Unicode|[[Aṣṭādhyāyī]]}}'', which is still in use today. {{Unicode|Pāṇini's}} [[grammar]] of Sanskrit is highly systematised and technical. Inherent in its analytic approach are the concepts of the [[phoneme]], the [[morpheme]] and the [[root]], only recognized by Western linguists some two millennia later. His rules fully describe Sanskrit morphology without any redundance. A consequence of his grammar's focus on brevity is its highly unintuitive structure, reminiscent of contemporary "machine language" (as opposed to "human readable" programming languages). His sophisticated logical rules and technique have been widely influential in ancient and modern linguistics.


The [[South India]]n linguist [[Tolkaappiyar|Tolkāppiyar]] (c. [[3rd century BC]]) wrote the ''[[Tolkāppiyam]]'', the grammar of [[Tamil language|Tamil]], which is also still in use today. [[Bhartrihari]] (c. [[450]]–[[510]]) was another important author on [[Indic]] linguistic theory.He theorized the act of speech as being made up of four stages: first, conceptualization of an idea, second, its verbalization and sequencing and third, delivery of speech into atmospheric air, all these by the speaker and last, the comprehension of speech by the listener, the  interpreter. The work of {{Unicode|Pāṇini}}, and the later Indian linguist Bhartrihari, had a significant influence on many of the foundational ideas proposed by [[Ferdinand de Saussure]], professor of Sanskrit, who is widely considered the father of modern structural linguistics.
The [[Sanskrit]] [[grammarian]] {{Unicode|[[Pāṇini]]}} (c. [[520 BC|520]] – [[460 BC]]) is the earliest known linguist and is often acknowledged as the founder of linguistics. He is most famous for formulating the 3,959 rules of Sanskrit [[Morphology (linguistics)|morphology]] in the text ''{{Unicode|[[Aṣṭādhyāyī]]}}'', which is still in use today. {{Unicode|Pāṇini's}} [[grammar]] of Sanskrit is highly systematised and technical. Inherent in its analytic approach are the concepts of the [[phoneme]], the [[morpheme]] and the [[root]], only recognized by Western linguists some two millennia later. His rules fully describe Sanskrit morphology without any redundance. A consequence of his grammar's focus on brevity is its highly unintuitive structure, reminiscent of contemporary "machine language" (as opposed to "human readable" programming languages). His sophisticated logical rules and technique have been widely influential in ancient and modern linguistics.
 
The [[South India]]n linguist [[Tolkaappiyar|Tolkāppiyar]] (c. [[3rd century BC]]) wrote the ''[[Tolkāppiyam]]'', the grammar of [[Tamil language|Tamil]], which is also still in use today. [[Bhartrihari]] (c. [[450]] [[510]]) was another important author on [[Indic]] linguistic theory. He theorized the act of speech as being made up of four stages: first, conceptualization of an idea, second, its verbalization and sequencing and third, delivery of speech into atmospheric air, all these by the speaker and last, the comprehension of speech by the listener, the  interpreter. The work of {{Unicode|Pāṇini}}, and the later Indian linguist Bhartrihari, had a significant influence on many of the foundational ideas proposed by [[Ferdinand de Saussure]], professor of Sanskrit, who is widely considered the father of modern structural linguistics.
 
In the [[Middle East]], the [[Persian language|Persian]] linguist [[Sibawayh]] made a detailed and professional description of [[Arabic language|Arabic]] in 760, in his monumental work, ''Al-kitab fi al-nahw'' (الكتاب في النحو, ''The Book on Grammar''), bringing many linguistic aspects of language to light. In his book he distinguished [[phonetics]] from [[phonology]].{{cn}}


Other early [[scholar]]s of linguistics include [[Jakob Grimm]], who devised the principle of consonantal shifts in pronunciation known as [[Grimm's Law]] in 1822, [[Karl Verner]], who discovered [[Verner's Law]], [[August Schleicher]] who created the "Stammbaumtheorie" and [[Johannes Schmidt (linguist)|Johannes Schmidt]] who developed the "Wellentheorie" ("wave model") in 1872. [[Ferdinand de Saussure]] was the founder of modern structural linguistics. [[Edward Sapir]], a leader in American structural linguistics, was one of the first who explored the relations between language studies and anthropology. His methodology had strong influence on all his successors. [[Noam Chomsky|Noam Chomsky's]] formal model of language, [[transformational-generative grammar]], developed under the influence of his teacher [[Zellig Harris]], who was in turn strongly influenced by [[Leonard Bloomfield]], has been the dominant one from the [[1960s]].  
Other early [[scholar]]s of linguistics include [[Jakob Grimm]], who devised the principle of consonantal shifts in pronunciation known as [[Grimm's Law]] in 1822, [[Karl Verner]], who discovered [[Verner's Law]], [[August Schleicher]] who created the "Stammbaumtheorie" and [[Johannes Schmidt (linguist)|Johannes Schmidt]] who developed the "Wellentheorie" ("wave model") in 1872. [[Ferdinand de Saussure]] was the founder of modern structural linguistics. [[Edward Sapir]], a leader in American structural linguistics, was one of the first who explored the relations between language studies and anthropology. His methodology had strong influence on all his successors. [[Noam Chomsky|Noam Chomsky's]] formal model of language, [[transformational-generative grammar]], developed under the influence of his teacher [[Zellig Harris]], who was in turn strongly influenced by [[Leonard Bloomfield]], has been the dominant one from the [[1960s]].  
Line 123: Line 134:
Chomsky remains by far the most influential linguist in the world today. Linguists working in frameworks such as [[Head-Driven Phrase Structure Grammar]] (HPSG) or [[Lexical Functional Grammar]] (LFG) stress the importance of formalization and formal rigor in linguistic description, and may distance themselves somewhat from Chomsky's more recent work (the "Minimalist" program for [[Transformational grammar]]), connecting more closely to earlier work of Chomsky's. Linguists working in [[Optimality Theory]] state generalizations in terms of violable rules, which is a greater departure from mainstream linguistics, and linguists working in various kinds of [[functional grammar]] and [[Cognitive Linguistics]] tend to stress the non-autonomy of linguistic knowledge and the non-universality of linguistic structures, thus departing importantly from the Chomskian paradigm.
Chomsky remains by far the most influential linguist in the world today. Linguists working in frameworks such as [[Head-Driven Phrase Structure Grammar]] (HPSG) or [[Lexical Functional Grammar]] (LFG) stress the importance of formalization and formal rigor in linguistic description, and may distance themselves somewhat from Chomsky's more recent work (the "Minimalist" program for [[Transformational grammar]]), connecting more closely to earlier work of Chomsky's. Linguists working in [[Optimality Theory]] state generalizations in terms of violable rules, which is a greater departure from mainstream linguistics, and linguists working in various kinds of [[functional grammar]] and [[Cognitive Linguistics]] tend to stress the non-autonomy of linguistic knowledge and the non-universality of linguistic structures, thus departing importantly from the Chomskian paradigm.


==Links==
== See also ==
===Interdisciplinary linguistic research===
{{wiktionary}}
* [[Anthropological linguistics]]
* [[List of basic linguistic topics]]
* [[Cognitive linguistics]]
* [[List of cognitive science topics]]
* [[Cognitive science]]
* [[List of linguistic topics]]
* [[Historical linguistics|Comparative linguistics]]
* [[Computational linguistics]]
** [[Machine translation]]
** [[Natural language processing]]
** [[Speaker recognition]] (authentication)
** [[Speech processing]]
** [[Speech recognition]]
** [[Speech synthesis]]
* [[Critical discourse analysis]]
* [[Cryptanalysis]]
* [[Decipherment]]
* [[Ecolinguistics]]
* [[Evolutionary linguistics]]
* [[Forensic linguistics]]
* [[Glottometrics]]
* [[Language acquisition]]
* [[Language attrition]]
* [[Language engineering]]
* [[Neurolinguistics]]
* [[Orthography]]
* [[Second language acquisition]]
* [[Stratificational linguistics]]
* [[Text linguistics]]
* [[Writing system]]s
 
===See also===
{{commonscat}}
{{commonscat}}
* [[Historical-comparative linguistics]]
<div style = "font-size:90%;">
* [[Integrational linguistics]]
{{Col-begin}}
* [[Lexicography]]
{{Col-1-of-2}}
* [[Lexicology]]
* [[Anthropological linguistics]]
* [[Corpus linguistics]]
* [[Cognitive linguistics]]
* [[Descriptive linguistics]]
* [[Cognitive science]]
* [[Linguistic typology]]
* [[Computational linguistics]]
* [[Semiotics]]
** [[Machine translation]]
** [[Natural language processing]]
** [[Speaker recognition]] (authentication)
** [[Speech processing]]
** [[Speech recognition]]
** [[Speech synthesis]]
* [[Concept Mining]]
* [[Corpus linguistics]]
* [[Critical discourse analysis]]
* [[Cryptanalysis]]
* [[Decipherment]]
* [[Descriptive linguistics]]
* [[Ecolinguistics]]
* [[Evolutionary linguistics]]
* [[Forensic linguistics]]
* [[Glottometrics]]
{{Col-2-of-2}}
* [[History of linguistics]]
* [[History of linguistics]]
* [[Intercultural competence]]
* [[Historical-comparative linguistics]]
* [[List of basic linguistic topics]]
* [[Integrational linguistics]]
* [[List of linguistic topics]]
* [[Intercultural competence]]
* [[Metacommunicative competence]]
* [[Language acquisition]]
* [[Philology]], the study of [[ancient]] [[text]]s and languages
* [[Language attrition]]
* [[Structuralism]]
* [[Language engineering]]
* [[List of cognitive science topics]]
* [[Lexicography]]/[[Lexicology]]
* [[Linguistic typology]]
* [[Metacommunicative competence]]
* [[Neurolinguistics]]
* [[Orthography]]
* [[Second language acquisition]]
* [[Semiotics]]
* [[Sociocultural linguistics]]
* [[Stratificational linguistics]]
* [[Structuralism]]
* [[Text linguistics]]
* [[Writing system]]s
{{Col-end}}
</div>


==References==
==References==
Line 176: Line 190:


===Textbooks===
===Textbooks===
* [[Jean Aitchison|'''Aitchison''', Jean]] (1995)  Linguistics: An Introduction. London: Hodder & Stoughton; London: Hodder Headline, 1999, 2nd Edition.
*{{cite book |last=Aitchison |first=Jean |title=Linguistics: An Introduction |origyear=1995 |edition=2nd |year=1999 |publisher=Hodder & Stoughton |location=London}}
* '''Akmajian, Adrian ''et al''''' (2001), ''Linguistics'', 5th ed., MIT Press. (ISBN 0-262-51123-1)
*{{cite book |last=Adrian |first=Akmajian |others=et al |title=Linguistics |year=2001 |publisher=MIT Press |id=ISBN 0-262-51123-1}}
* '''Sergiusz Griniewicz, Elwira M. Dubieniec'''. Introduction To Linguistics. 2nd ed. Białystok, WSFiZ, 2004, 91 s.
*{{cite book |last=Griniewicz |first=Sergiusz |coauthors=Elwira M. Dubieniec |title=Introduction To Linguistics |edition=2nd |year=2004 |publisher=Białystok, WSFiZ |pages=91}}
* '''Hudson''', G. (2000)  ''Essential Introductory Linguistics''. Oxford: Blackwell.
* Hudson, G. (2000)  ''Essential Introductory Linguistics''. Oxford: Blackwell.
* '''Lyons, John''' (1995), ''Linguistic Semantics'', Cambridge University Press. (ISBN 0-521-43877-2)
* Lyons, John (1995), ''Linguistic Semantics'', Cambridge University Press. (ISBN 0-521-43877-2)
* '''Napoli''', Donna J. (2003) ''Language Matters. A Guide to Everyday Questions about Language''. Oxford University Press.
* Napoli, Donna J. (2003) ''Language Matters. A Guide to Everyday Questions about Language''. Oxford University Press.
* '''O'Grady, William D., Michael Dobrovolsky & Francis Katamba [eds.]''' (2001), ''Contemporary Linguistics'', Longman. (ISBN 0-582-24691-1) - Lower Level
* O'Grady, William D., Michael Dobrovolsky & Francis Katamba [eds.] (2001), ''Contemporary Linguistics'', Longman. (ISBN 0-582-24691-1) - Lower Level
* '''Taylor, John R.''' (2003), ''Cognitive Grammar'', Oxford University Press. (ISBN 0-19-870033-4)
* Taylor, John R. (2003), ''Cognitive Grammar'', Oxford University Press. (ISBN 0-19-870033-4)
* '''Trask''', R. L. (1995)  ''Language: The Basics''. London: Routledge.
* Trask, R. L. (1995)  ''Language: The Basics''. London: Routledge.
* '''Ungerer, Friedrich & Hans-Jorg Schmid''' (1996), ''An Introduction to Cognitive Linguistics'', Longman. (ISBN 0-582-23966-4)
* Ungerer, Friedrich & Hans-Jorg Schmid (1996), ''An Introduction to Cognitive Linguistics'', Longman. (ISBN 0-582-23966-4)


===Academic works===
===Academic works===
* '''[[Gilles Fauconnier|Fauconnier, Gilles]]'''
* [[Gilles Fauconnier|Fauconnier, Gilles]]
** (1995), ''Mental Spaces'', 2nd ed., Cambridge University Press. (ISBN 0-521-44949-9)
** (1995), ''Mental Spaces'', 2nd ed., Cambridge University Press. (ISBN 0-521-44949-9)
** (1997), ''Mappings in Thought and Language'', Cambridge University Press. (ISBN 0-521-59953-9)
** (1997), ''Mappings in Thought and Language'', Cambridge University Press. (ISBN 0-521-59953-9)
** '''& [[Mark Turner (cognitive scientist)|Mark Turner]]''' (2003), ''The Way We Think'', Basic Books. (ISBN 0-465-08786-8)
** & [[Mark Turner (cognitive scientist)|Mark Turner]] (2003), ''The Way We Think'', Basic Books. (ISBN 0-465-08786-8)
*** Rymer, p. 48, quoted in Fauconnier and Turner, p. 353
*** Rymer, p. 48, quoted in Fauconnier and Turner, p. 353
* '''[[Geoffrey Sampson|Sampson, Geoffrey]] ''' (1982), ''Schools of Linguistics'', Stanford University Press. (ISBN 0-8047-1125-9)
* [[Geoffrey Sampson|Sampson, Geoffrey]] (1982), ''Schools of Linguistics'', Stanford University Press. (ISBN 0-8047-1125-9)
* '''Sweetser, Eve''' (1992), ''From Etymology to Pragmatics'', repr ed., Cambridge University Press. (ISBN 0-521-42442-9)
* Sweetser, Eve (1992), ''From Etymology to Pragmatics'', repr ed., Cambridge University Press. (ISBN 0-521-42442-9)


===Popular works===
===Popular works===
* '''Bloomfield, Leonard'''. ''Language''.
* Bloomfield, Leonard. ''Language''.
*'''[[Anthony Burgess|Burgess, Anthony]]'''
*[[Anthony Burgess|Burgess, Anthony]]
** (1964), ''[[Language Made Plain]]
** (1964), ''[[Language Made Plain]]
** (1992), ''[[A Mouthful of Air]]
** (1992), ''[[A Mouthful of Air]]
* '''Deacon, Terrence''' (1998), ''The Symbolic Species'', WW Norton & Co. (ISBN 0-393-31754-4)
* Deacon, Terrence (1998), ''The Symbolic Species'', WW Norton & Co. (ISBN 0-393-31754-4)
* '''Deutscher, Guy, Dr.''' (2005), ''The Unfolding of Language'', Metropolitan Books (ISBN 0-8050-7907-6) (ISBN 9780805079074
* Deutscher, Guy, Dr. (2005), ''The Unfolding of Language'', Metropolitan Books (ISBN 0-8050-7907-6) (ISBN 978-0-8050-7907-4
* '''Hayakawa, Alan R & S. I.''' (1990), ''Language in Thought and Action'', Harvest. (ISBN 0-15-648240-1)
* Harrison, K. David. (2007) When Languages Die: The Extinction of the World's Languages and the Erosion of Human Knowledge. New York and London: Oxford University Press.
* '''[[Steven Pinker|Pinker, Steven]]'''
* Hayakawa, Alan R & S. I. (1990), ''Language in Thought and Action'', Harvest. (ISBN 0-15-648240-1)
* [[Steven Pinker|Pinker, Steven]]
** (2000), ''[[The Language Instinct]]'', repr ed., Perennial. (ISBN 0-06-095833-2)
** (2000), ''[[The Language Instinct]]'', repr ed., Perennial. (ISBN 0-06-095833-2)
** (2000), ''Words and Rules'', Perennial. (ISBN 0-06-095840-5)
** (2000), ''Words and Rules'', Perennial. (ISBN 0-06-095840-5)
* '''Rymer, Russ''' (1992), ''Annals of Science'' in "[http://newyorker.com/ The New Yorker]", 13th April
* Rymer, Russ (1992), ''Annals of Science'' in "[http://newyorker.com/ The New Yorker]", 13th April
* '''Sapir, Edward'''. ''Language''.
* Sapir, Edward. ''Language''.
* '''Saussure, Ferdinand de'''. ''Cours de linguistique générale'' [''Course in general linguistics''].
* [[Saussure, Ferdinand de]]. ''[[Course in general linguistics|Cours de linguistique générale]]''.
* '''White, Lydia''' (1992), ''Universal Grammar and Second Language Acquisition''.
* White, Lydia (1992), ''Universal Grammar and Second Language Acquisition''.


===Reference books===
===Reference books===
* '''Aronoff, Mark & Janie Rees-Miller''' (Eds.) (2003) ''The Handbook of Linguistics''. Blackwell Publishers. (ISBN 1-4051-0252-7)
* Aronoff, Mark & Janie Rees-Miller (Eds.) (2003) ''The Handbook of Linguistics''. Blackwell Publishers. (ISBN 1-4051-0252-7)
* '''Asher''', R. (Ed.) (1993) ''Encyclopedia of Language and Linguistics''. Oxford: Pergamon Press. 10 vols.
* Asher, R. (Ed.) (1993) ''Encyclopedia of Language and Linguistics''. Oxford: Pergamon Press. 10 vols.
* '''Bright''', William (Ed) (1992)  ''International Encyclopedia of Linguistics''. Oxford University Press. 4 Vols.
* Bright, William (Ed) (1992)  ''International Encyclopedia of Linguistics''. Oxford University Press. 4 Vols.
* '''Brown''', Keith R. (Ed.) (2005) ''Encyclopedia of Language and Linguistics'' (2nd ed.). Elsevier. 14 vols.
* Brown, Keith R. (Ed.) (2005) ''Encyclopedia of Language and Linguistics'' (2nd ed.). Elsevier. 14 vols.
* '''Bussmann''', H. (1996)  ''Routledge Dictionary of Language and Linguistics''. Routledge (translated from German).
* Bussmann, H. (1996)  ''Routledge Dictionary of Language and Linguistics''. Routledge (translated from German).
* '''Crystal''', David
* [[David Crystal|Crystal, David]]
** (1987) ''The Cambridge Encyclopaedia of Language''.  Cambridge University Press.
** (1987) ''The Cambridge Encyclopaedia of Language''.  Cambridge University Press.
** (1991) ''A Dictionary of Linguistics and Phonetics''. Blackwell.  (ISBN 0-631-17871-6)
** (1991) ''A Dictionary of Linguistics and Phonetics''. Blackwell.  (ISBN 0-631-17871-6)
** (1992) ''An Encyclopaedic Dictionary of Language and Languages''.  Oxford: Blackwell.
** (1992) ''An Encyclopaedic Dictionary of Language and Languages''.  Oxford: Blackwell.
* '''Frawley''', William (Ed.) (2003) ''International Encyclopedia of Linguistics'' (2nd ed.). Oxford University Press.
* Frawley, William (Ed.) (2003) ''International Encyclopedia of Linguistics'' (2nd ed.). Oxford University Press.
* '''Malmkjaer''', Kirsten (1991) ''The Linguistics Encyclopaedia''. Routledge (ISBN 0-415-22210-9)
* Malmkjaer, Kirsten (1991) ''The Linguistics Encyclopaedia''. Routledge (ISBN 0-415-22210-9)
* '''Trask''', R. L.
* Trask, R. L.
**  (1993) ''A Dictionary of Grammatical Terms in Linguistics''.  Routledge.  (ISBN 0-415-08628-0)
**  (1993) ''A Dictionary of Grammatical Terms in Linguistics''.  Routledge.  (ISBN 0-415-08628-0)
**  (1996) ''Dictionary of Phonetics and Phonology''. Routledge.
**  (1996) ''Dictionary of Phonetics and Phonology''. Routledge.
Line 232: Line 247:
==External links==
==External links==
{{Wikibookspar|Wikiversity|School of Linguistics}}
{{Wikibookspar|Wikiversity|School of Linguistics}}
* [http://www.lsadc.org/info/ling-fields.cfm subfields according to the Linguistic Society of America]
* [http://www.lsadc.org/info/ling-fields.cfm Subfields according to the Linguistic Society of America]
* [http://www.sil.org/linguistics/GlossaryOfLinguisticTerms/index.htm Glossary of linguistic terms] and [http://www.sil.org/linguistics/glossary_fe/ French<->English glossary] at [[SIL International]]
* [http://www.sil.org/linguistics/GlossaryOfLinguisticTerms/index.htm Glossary of linguistic terms] and [http://www.sil.org/linguistics/glossary_fe/ French<->English glossary] at [[SIL International]]
* [http://www.unizar.es/departamentos/filologia_inglesa/garciala/bibliography.html "Linguistics" section] of A Bibliography of Literary Theory, Criticism and Philology, ed. J. A. García Landa (University of Zaragoza, Spain)
* [http://www.unizar.es/departamentos/filologia_inglesa/garciala/bibliography.html "Linguistics" section] of A Bibliography of Literary Theory, Criticism and Philology, ed. J. A. García Landa (University of Zaragoza, Spain)
* [http://www.specgram.com/CL.1/06.schadenpoodle.guide1.html A Preliminary Field Guide to Linguists, Part One]: A humorous overview of the various branches of linguistics and their practitioners. There is also a [http://www.specgram.com/CL.2/05.schadenpoodle.guide2.html Part Two].
* [http://www.linguistlist.org The Linguist List]
* [http://www.lingforum.com Linguistics Forum]
* [http://lingformant.vertebratesilence.com Lingformant: The Science of Linguistics in the News]
* [http://www2.let.uu.nl/UiL-OTS/Lexicon/ Lexicon of Linguistics created by Johan Kerstens, Eddy Ruys and Joost Zwarts at the Utrecht institute of Linguistics OTS, university of Utrecht]
* [http://www.linguistics-journal.com/ Linguistics Research Journal]
* [http://www.linguistics-online.com/ The Virtual Linguistics Campus, The world's largest E-Learning Platform for Linguistics]
* [http://www.gestalttheory.net/linguistics/ Gestalt Theory and Linguistics]
* [http://www.zompist.com/langfaq.html The sci.lang FAQ - Frequently asked questions about linguistics]
===Representation of speech===
* [[International Phonetic Alphabet]] (IPA), a system used to write down and reproduce the [[sound]]s of [[human speech]].
* [[SAMPA]], an [[ASCII]]-only transcription for the IPA used by some authors. See also [http://www.phon.ucl.ac.uk/home/sampa/home.htm the SAMPA home page].


{{Social sciences-footer}}
{{Social sciences-footer}}
Line 255: Line 257:
[[af:Taalwetenskappe]]
[[af:Taalwetenskappe]]
[[ar:لسانيات]]
[[ar:لسانيات]]
[[an:Lingüística]]
[[an:Lingüistica]]
[[ast:Llingüística]]
[[ast:Llingüística]]
[[bm:Kankalan]]
[[bm:Kankalan]]
Line 284: Line 286:
[[ko:언어학]]
[[ko:언어학]]
[[hi:भाषाविज्ञान]]
[[hi:भाषाविज्ञान]]
[[hsb:Rěčespyt]]
[[hr:Jezikoslovlje]]
[[hr:Jezikoslovlje]]
[[io:Linguistiko]]
[[io:Linguistiko]]
Line 307: Line 310:
[[mt:Lingwistika]]
[[mt:Lingwistika]]
[[mo:Лингвистикэ]]
[[mo:Лингвистикэ]]
[[nah:Tlahtolmatiliztli]]
[[nah:Tlahtōlmatiliztli]]
[[nl:Taalkunde]]
[[nl:Taalkunde]]
[[ja:言語学]]
[[ja:言語学]]
[[no:Lingvistikk]]
[[no:Lingvistikk]]
[[nn:Lingvistikk]]
[[nn:Lingvistikk]]
[[nrm:Lîndgistique]]
[[nov:Linguistike]]
[[nds:Spraakwetenschop]]
[[nds:Spraakwetenschop]]
[[pl:Językoznawstwo]]
[[pl:Językoznawstwo]]
Line 329: Line 334:
[[su:Linguistik]]
[[su:Linguistik]]
[[fi:Kielitiede]]
[[fi:Kielitiede]]
[[sv:Lingvistik]]
[[sv:Språkvetenskap]]
[[tl:Linggwistika]]
[[tl:Linggwistika]]
[[ta:மொழியியல்]]
[[ta:மொழியியல்]]
Line 336: Line 341:
[[vi:Ngôn ngữ học]]
[[vi:Ngôn ngữ học]]
[[tg:Забоншиносӣ]]
[[tg:Забоншиносӣ]]
[[tg:Зобоншеноси]]
[[tr:Dil bilimi]]
[[tr:Dil bilimi]]
[[uk:Мовознавство]]
[[uk:Мовознавство]]
Line 342: Line 346:
[[vec:Łenguisdega]]
[[vec:Łenguisdega]]
[[fiu-vro:Keeletiidüs]]
[[fiu-vro:Keeletiidüs]]
[[wa:Linwince]]
[[zh:语言学]]
[[zh:语言学]]
[[zh-classical:語言學]]
[[Category:Linguistics Workgroup (Top)]]
[[Category:Linguistics Workgroup (Top)]]

Revision as of 21:28, 15 December 2006

Linguistics
Phonology
Syntax
Morphology
Semantics
Pragmatics
Theoretical linguistics
Generative linguistics
Cognitive linguistics
Language acquisition
First language acquisition
Second language acquisition
Applied linguistics
Psycholinguistics
Phonetics
Sociolinguistics
Creolistics
Evolutionary linguistics
Linguistic variation
Linguistic typology
Anthropological linguistics
Computational linguistics
Descriptive linguistics
Historical linguistics
Comparative linguistics
History of linguistics
Languagenaturalconstructed
Grammar

Linguistics is the scientific study of language. Someone who engages in this study is called a linguist. Linguistics can be theoretical or applied.

Theoretical (or general) linguistics studies language structure (grammar), and meaning (semantics). The study of grammar encompasses morphology (formation and alteration of words) and syntax (the rules that determine the way words combine into phrases and sentences). Phonology, which is the system used to represent language through abstract 'sounds', also forms part of this field.

Linguistics compares languages (comparative linguistics) and explores their histories, in order to find universal properties of language and to account for its development and origins (historical linguistics). Slightly separate from general linguistics is the sub-field of phonetics, the study of how sounds are produced and perceived.

Applied linguistics puts linguistic theories into practice in areas such as foreign language teaching, speech therapy, translation and speech pathology.

Linguistic inquiry is pursued by a wide variety of specialists, who may not all be in harmonious agreement; as Russ Rymer flamboyantly puts it:

Linguistics is arguably the most hotly contested property in the academic realm. It is soaked with the blood of poets, theologians, philosophers, philologists, psychologists, biologists, anthropologists, and neurologists, along with whatever blood can be got out of grammarians. 1

Divisions, specialties, and subfields

The central concern of theoretical linguistics is to characterize the nature of human language ability, or competence: to explain what it is that an individual knows when said to know a language; and to explain how it is that individuals come to know languages.

All humans (setting aside extremely pathological cases) achieve competence in whatever language is spoken (or signed, in the case of sign language) around them when they are growing up, with apparently little need for conscious instruction. Non-humans do not. Therefore, there is some basic innate property of humans that causes them to be able to use language. There is no discernable genetic process responsible for differences between languages: an individual will acquire whatever language(s) they are exposed to as a child, regardless of their parentage or ethnic origin.

Linguistic structures are pairings of meaning and sound (or other externalization). Linguists may specialize in some subpart of the linguistic structure, which can be arranged in the following terms, from sound to meaning:

  • Phonetics, the study of the sounds of human language
  • Phonology (or phonemics), the study of patterns of a language's basic sounds
  • Morphology, the study of the internal structure of words
  • Syntax, the study of how words combine to form grammatical sentences
  • Semantics, the study of the meaning of words (lexical semantics) and fixed word combinations (phraseology), and how these combine to form the meanings of sentences
  • Pragmatics, the study of how utterances are used (literally, figuratively, or otherwise) in communicative acts
  • Discourse analysis, the study of sentences organised into texts

The independent significance of each of these areas is not universally acknowledged, however, and many linguists would agree that the divisions overlap considerably. Nevertheless, each area has core concepts that foster significant scholarly inquiry and research.

Intersecting with these specialty domains are fields arranged around the kind of external factors that are considered. For example

Variation

A substantial part of linguistic investigation is into the nature of the differences among the languages of the world. The nature of variation is very important to an understanding of human linguistic ability in general: if human linguistic ability is very narrowly constrained by biological properties of the species, then languages must be very similar. If human linguistic ability is unconstrained, then languages might vary greatly.

But there are different ways to interpret similarities among languages. For example, the Latin language spoken by the Romans developed into Spanish in Spain and Italian in Italy. Similarities between Spanish and Italian are in many cases due to both being descended from Latin. So in principle, if two languages share some property, this property might either be due to common inheritance or due to some property of the human language faculty.

Often, the possibility of common inheritance can be essentially ruled out. Given the fact that learning language comes quite easily to humans, it can be assumed that languages have been spoken at least as long as there have been biologically modern humans, probably at least fifty thousand years. Independent measures of language change (for example, comparing the language of ancient texts to the daughter languages spoken today) suggest that change is rapid enough to make it impossible to reconstruct a language that was spoken so long ago; as a consequence of this, common features of languages spoken in different parts of the world are not normally taken as evidence for common ancestry.

Even more striking, there are documented cases of sign languages being developed in communities of congenitally deaf people who could not have been exposed to spoken language. The properties of these sign languages have been shown to conform generally to many of the properties of spoken languages, strengthening the hypothesis that those properties are not due to common ancestry but to more general characteristics of the way languages are learned.

Loosely speaking, the collection of properties which all languages share can be referred to as "universal grammar" (or UG). However, there is much debate around this topic and the term is used in several different ways.

Universal properties of language may be partly due to universal aspects of human experience; for example all humans experience water, and the fact that all human languages have a word for water is probably not unrelated to this fact. The challenging questions regarding universal grammar generally require one to control this factor. Clearly, experience is part of the process by which individuals learn languages. But experience by itself is not enough, since animals raised around people learn extremely little human language, if any at all.

A more interesting example is this: suppose that all human languages distinguish nouns from verbs (this is generally believed to be true). This would require a more sophisticated explanation, since nouns and verbs do not exist in the world, apart from languages that make use of them.

In general, a property of UG could be due to general properties of human cognition, or due to some property of human cognition that is specific to language. Too little is understood about human cognition in general to allow a meaningful distinction to be made. As a result, generalizations are often stated in theoretical linguistics without a stand being taken on whether the generalization could have some bearing on other aspects of cognition.

Properties of language

It has been understood since the time of the ancient Greeks that languages tend to be organized around grammatical categories such as noun and verb, nominative and accusative, or present and past. The vocabulary and grammar of a language are organized around these fundamental categories.

In addition to making substantial use of discrete categories, language has the important property that it organizes elements into recursive structures; this allows, for example, a noun phrase to contain another noun phrase (as in the chimpanzee's lips) or a clause to contain a clause (as in I think that it's raining). Though recursion in grammar was implicitly recognized much earlier (for example by Jespersen), the importance of this aspect of language was only fully realized after the 1957 publication of Noam Chomsky's book Syntactic Structures,[1] which presented a formal grammar of a fragment of English. Prior to this, the most detailed descriptions of linguistic systems were of phonological or morphological systems, which tend to be closed and admit little creativity.

Chomsky used a context-free grammar augmented with transformations. Since then, context-free grammars have been written for substantial fragments of various languages (for example GPSG, for English), but it has been demonstrated that human languages include cross-serial dependencies, which cannot be handled adequately by Context-free grammars. This requires increased power, for example transformations.

An example of a natural-language clause involving a cross-serial dependency is the Dutch[2][3]

Ik denk dat Jan Piet de kinderen zag helpen zwemmen
I think that Jan Piet the children saw help swim
'I think that Jan saw Piet help the children swim'

The important point is that the noun phrases before the verb cluster (Jan, Piet, de kinderen) are identified with the verbs in the verb cluster (zag, helpen, zwemmen) in left-right order.

This means that natural language formalisms must be relatively powerful in terms of generative capacity. The models currently used (LFG, HPSG, Minimalism) are very powerful, in general too powerful to be computationally tractable in principle. Implementations of them are scaled down.

Details on selected divisions and subfields

Contextual linguistics

Contextual linguistics may include the study of linguistics in interaction with other academic disciplines. Whereas in core theoretical linguistics language is studied for its own sake, the interdisciplinary areas of linguistics consider how language interacts with the rest of the world.

Sociolinguistics, anthropological linguistics, and linguistic anthropology are social sciences that consider the interactions between linguistics and society as a whole.

Critical discourse analysis is where rhetoric and philosophy interact with linguistics.

Psycholinguistics and neurolinguistics combine medical science and linguistics.

Other cross-disciplinary areas of linguistics include language acquisition, evolutionary linguistics, computational linguistics and cognitive science.

Applied linguistics

Whereas theoretical linguistics is concerned with finding and describing generalities both within particular languages and among all languages, applied linguistics takes the results of those findings and applies them to other areas. Often applied linguistics refers to the use of linguistic research in language teaching, but results of linguistic research are used in many other areas, as well.

Many areas of applied linguistics today involve the explicit use of computers. Speech synthesis and speech recognition use phonetic and phonemic knowledge to provide voice interfaces to computers. Applications of computational linguistics in machine translation, computer-assisted translation, and natural language processing are extremely fruitful areas of applied linguistics which have come to the forefront in recent years with increasing computing power. Their influence has had a great effect on theories of syntax and semantics, as modelling syntactic and semantic theories on computers constrains the theories to computable operations and provides a more rigorous mathematical basis.

Today, the term 'applied linguistics' is used mostly to refer to "second language acquisition." Top applied linguistics programs are usually the ones that have good emphasis on second language acquisition either from linguistic or cognitive point of view.

Diachronic linguistics

Whereas the core of theoretical linguistics is concerned with studying languages at a particular point in time (usually the present), diachronic linguistics examines how language changes through time, sometimes over centuries. Historical linguistics enjoys both a rich history (the study of linguistics grew out of historical linguistics) and a strong theoretical foundation for the study of language change.

In universities in the United States, the non-historic perspective seems to have the upper hand. Many introductory linguistics classes, for example, cover historical linguistics only cursorily. The shift in focus to a non-historic perspective started with Saussure and became predominant with Noam Chomsky.

Explicitly historical perspectives include historical-comparative linguistics and etymology.

Prescription and description

Main article: Prescription and description.

Research currently performed under the name "linguistics" is purely descriptive; linguists seek to clarify the nature of language without passing value judgments or trying to chart future language directions. Nonetheless, there are many professionals and amateurs who also prescribe rules of language, holding a particular standard out for all to follow.

Prescriptivists tend to be found among the ranks of language educators and journalists, and not in the actual academic discipline of linguistics. They hold clear notions of what is right and wrong, and may assign themselves the responsibility of ensuring that the next generation use the variety of language that is most likely to lead to "success," often the acrolect of a particular language. The reasons for their intolerance of "incorrect usage" may include distrust of neologisms, connections to socially-disapproved dialects (i.e., basilects), or simple conflicts with pet theories. An extreme version of prescriptivism can be found among censors, whose personal mission is to eradicate words and structures which they consider to be destructive to society.

Descriptivists, on the other hand, do not accept the prescriptivists' notion of "incorrect usage." They might describe the usages the other has in mind simply as "idiosyncratic," or they may discover a regularity (a rule) that the usage in question follows (in contrast to the common prescriptive assumption that "bad" usage is unsystematic). Within the context of fieldwork, descriptive linguistics refers to the study of language using a descriptivist approach. Descriptivist methodology more closely resembles scientific methodology in other disciplines.

Speech versus writing

Most contemporary linguists work under the assumption that spoken language is more fundamental, and thus more important to study than written language. Reasons for this perspective include:

  • Speech appears to be a human universal, whereas there have been many cultures and speech communities that lack written communication;
  • People learn to speak and process spoken languages more easily and much earlier than writing;
  • A number of cognitive scientists argue that the brain has an innate "language module", knowledge of which is thought to come more from studying speech than writing, particularly since language as speech is held to be an evolutionary adaptation, whereas writing is a comparatively recent invention.

Of course, linguists agree that the study of written language can be worthwhile and valuable. For linguistic research that uses the methods of corpus linguistics and computational linguistics, written language is often much more convenient for processing large amounts of linguistic data. Large corpora of spoken language are difficult to create and hard to find, and are typically transcribed and written. Additionally, linguists have turned to text-based discourse occurring in various formats of computer-mediated communication as a viable site for linguistic inquiry.

The study of writing systems themselves is in any case considered a branch of linguistics.

History of linguistics

For more information, see: History of linguistics.

Early Indian Vedic texts (Rig Veda 1:164:45; 4:58:3; 10:125) suggest a structure for languages: Language is composed of sentences with four stages of evolution that are expressed in three tenses (past, present and future). The sentences are composed of words that have two distinct forms of existence (vocal form, the word, and perceptional form, the meaning). These words are recognized mainly as verbs that represent real world acts and nouns that take on seven* cases (depending on their mode of participation in real world acts). (* The number, seven, here is not very critical; the message is that the nouns are inflected into appropriate cases to indicate their mode of participation in concerned acts).

The Sanskrit grammarian Pāṇini (c. 520460 BC) is the earliest known linguist and is often acknowledged as the founder of linguistics. He is most famous for formulating the 3,959 rules of Sanskrit morphology in the text Aṣṭādhyāyī, which is still in use today. Pāṇini's grammar of Sanskrit is highly systematised and technical. Inherent in its analytic approach are the concepts of the phoneme, the morpheme and the root, only recognized by Western linguists some two millennia later. His rules fully describe Sanskrit morphology without any redundance. A consequence of his grammar's focus on brevity is its highly unintuitive structure, reminiscent of contemporary "machine language" (as opposed to "human readable" programming languages). His sophisticated logical rules and technique have been widely influential in ancient and modern linguistics.

The South Indian linguist Tolkāppiyar (c. 3rd century BC) wrote the Tolkāppiyam, the grammar of Tamil, which is also still in use today. Bhartrihari (c. 450510) was another important author on Indic linguistic theory. He theorized the act of speech as being made up of four stages: first, conceptualization of an idea, second, its verbalization and sequencing and third, delivery of speech into atmospheric air, all these by the speaker and last, the comprehension of speech by the listener, the interpreter. The work of Pāṇini, and the later Indian linguist Bhartrihari, had a significant influence on many of the foundational ideas proposed by Ferdinand de Saussure, professor of Sanskrit, who is widely considered the father of modern structural linguistics.

In the Middle East, the Persian linguist Sibawayh made a detailed and professional description of Arabic in 760, in his monumental work, Al-kitab fi al-nahw (الكتاب في النحو, The Book on Grammar), bringing many linguistic aspects of language to light. In his book he distinguished phonetics from phonology.Template:Cn

Other early scholars of linguistics include Jakob Grimm, who devised the principle of consonantal shifts in pronunciation known as Grimm's Law in 1822, Karl Verner, who discovered Verner's Law, August Schleicher who created the "Stammbaumtheorie" and Johannes Schmidt who developed the "Wellentheorie" ("wave model") in 1872. Ferdinand de Saussure was the founder of modern structural linguistics. Edward Sapir, a leader in American structural linguistics, was one of the first who explored the relations between language studies and anthropology. His methodology had strong influence on all his successors. Noam Chomsky's formal model of language, transformational-generative grammar, developed under the influence of his teacher Zellig Harris, who was in turn strongly influenced by Leonard Bloomfield, has been the dominant one from the 1960s.

Chomsky remains by far the most influential linguist in the world today. Linguists working in frameworks such as Head-Driven Phrase Structure Grammar (HPSG) or Lexical Functional Grammar (LFG) stress the importance of formalization and formal rigor in linguistic description, and may distance themselves somewhat from Chomsky's more recent work (the "Minimalist" program for Transformational grammar), connecting more closely to earlier work of Chomsky's. Linguists working in Optimality Theory state generalizations in terms of violable rules, which is a greater departure from mainstream linguistics, and linguists working in various kinds of functional grammar and Cognitive Linguistics tend to stress the non-autonomy of linguistic knowledge and the non-universality of linguistic structures, thus departing importantly from the Chomskian paradigm.

See also

Template:Wiktionary

Template:Commonscat

Template:Col-1-of-2Template:Col-2-of-2

References

  1. Chomsky, Noam. 1957. "Syntactic Structures". Mouton, the Hague.
  2. Bresnan, Joan, Ronald Kaplan, Stanley Peters, and Annie Zaenen. 1982. Cross-serial dependencies in Dutch. Linguistic Inquiry 13:613-636.
  3. Shieber, Stuart. 1985. Evidence against the context-freeness of natural language. Linguistics and Philosophy 8:333-344.

Textbooks

  • Aitchison, Jean [1995] (1999). Linguistics: An Introduction, 2nd. London: Hodder & Stoughton. 
  • Adrian, Akmajian (2001). Linguistics, et al. MIT Press. ISBN 0-262-51123-1. 
  • Griniewicz, Sergiusz; Elwira M. Dubieniec (2004). Introduction To Linguistics, 2nd. Białystok, WSFiZ, 91. 
  • Hudson, G. (2000) Essential Introductory Linguistics. Oxford: Blackwell.
  • Lyons, John (1995), Linguistic Semantics, Cambridge University Press. (ISBN 0-521-43877-2)
  • Napoli, Donna J. (2003) Language Matters. A Guide to Everyday Questions about Language. Oxford University Press.
  • O'Grady, William D., Michael Dobrovolsky & Francis Katamba [eds.] (2001), Contemporary Linguistics, Longman. (ISBN 0-582-24691-1) - Lower Level
  • Taylor, John R. (2003), Cognitive Grammar, Oxford University Press. (ISBN 0-19-870033-4)
  • Trask, R. L. (1995) Language: The Basics. London: Routledge.
  • Ungerer, Friedrich & Hans-Jorg Schmid (1996), An Introduction to Cognitive Linguistics, Longman. (ISBN 0-582-23966-4)

Academic works

Popular works

Reference books

  • Aronoff, Mark & Janie Rees-Miller (Eds.) (2003) The Handbook of Linguistics. Blackwell Publishers. (ISBN 1-4051-0252-7)
  • Asher, R. (Ed.) (1993) Encyclopedia of Language and Linguistics. Oxford: Pergamon Press. 10 vols.
  • Bright, William (Ed) (1992) International Encyclopedia of Linguistics. Oxford University Press. 4 Vols.
  • Brown, Keith R. (Ed.) (2005) Encyclopedia of Language and Linguistics (2nd ed.). Elsevier. 14 vols.
  • Bussmann, H. (1996) Routledge Dictionary of Language and Linguistics. Routledge (translated from German).
  • Crystal, David
    • (1987) The Cambridge Encyclopaedia of Language. Cambridge University Press.
    • (1991) A Dictionary of Linguistics and Phonetics. Blackwell. (ISBN 0-631-17871-6)
    • (1992) An Encyclopaedic Dictionary of Language and Languages. Oxford: Blackwell.
  • Frawley, William (Ed.) (2003) International Encyclopedia of Linguistics (2nd ed.). Oxford University Press.
  • Malmkjaer, Kirsten (1991) The Linguistics Encyclopaedia. Routledge (ISBN 0-415-22210-9)
  • Trask, R. L.
    • (1993) A Dictionary of Grammatical Terms in Linguistics. Routledge. (ISBN 0-415-08628-0)
    • (1996) Dictionary of Phonetics and Phonology. Routledge.
    • (1997) A student's dictionary of language and linguistics.
    • (1999) Key Concepts in Language and Linguistics. London: Routledge.

External links

Template:Wikibookspar

Template:Social sciences-footer

af:Taalwetenskappe ar:لسانيات an:Lingüistica ast:Llingüística bm:Kankalan bn:ভাষাবিজ্ঞান zh-min-nan:Gí-giân-ha̍k be:Мовазнаўства br:Yezhoniezh bg:Езикознание ca:Lingüística cv:Лингвистика ceb:Linggwistiks cs:Lingvistika co:Linguistica cy:Ieithyddiaeth da:Sprogforskning de:Sprachwissenschaft et:Keeleteadus el:Γλωσσολογία es:Lingüística eo:Lingvistiko eu:Hizkuntzalaritza fa:زبانشناسی fr:Linguistique fy:Taalkunde fur:Lenghistiche ga:Teangeolaíocht gl:Lingüística ko:언어학 hi:भाषाविज्ञान hsb:Rěčespyt hr:Jezikoslovlje io:Linguistiko id:Linguistik ia:Linguistica ie:Linguistica os:Æвзагзонынад is:Málvísindi it:Linguistica he:בלשנות ka:ენათმეცნიერება csb:Lingwistika kw:Scyens Yeth ku:Zimannasî lad:Linguistika la:Linguistica lv:Valodniecība lb:Sproochwëssenschaft lt:Kalbotyra li:Taalweitesjap hu:Nyelvészet mk:Лингвистика mt:Lingwistika mo:Лингвистикэ nah:Tlahtōlmatiliztli nl:Taalkunde ja:言語学 no:Lingvistikk nn:Lingvistikk nrm:Lîndgistique nov:Linguistike nds:Spraakwetenschop pl:Językoznawstwo pt:Lingüística ro:Lingvistică rmy:Chhibavipen ru:Лингвистика sc:Linguìstica sco:Lingueistics sq:Gjuhësia scn:Linguìstica simple:Linguistics sk:Jazykoveda sl:Jezikoslovje sr:Лингвистика sh:Jezikoslovlje su:Linguistik fi:Kielitiede sv:Språkvetenskap tl:Linggwistika ta:மொழியியல் tt:Tel beleme th:ภาษาศาสตร์ vi:Ngôn ngữ học tg:Забоншиносӣ tr:Dil bilimi uk:Мовознавство ur:لسانيات vec:Łenguisdega fiu-vro:Keeletiidüs wa:Linwince zh:语言学 zh-classical:語言學